Cognitive enhancements

From PsychonautWiki
Jump to: navigation, search

Cognitive enhancements can be defined as any subjective effect which increases or raises the intensity of a facet of a person's cognition.

This page lists and describes the various cognitive enhancements which can occur under the influence of certain psychoactive compounds.

Analysis enhancement

Main article: Analysis enhancement

Analysis enhancement is a perceived improvement of a person's overall ability to logically process information[1][2][3] or creatively analyze concepts, ideas, and scenarios. This effect can lead to a deep state of contemplation which often results in an abundance of new and insightful ideas. It can give the person a perceived ability to better analyze concepts and problems in a manner which allows them to reach new conclusions, perspectives, and solutions which would have been otherwise difficult to conceive of.

Although this effect will often result in deep states of introspection, in other cases it can produce states which are not introspective but instead result in a deep analysis of the exterior world, both taken as a whole and as the things which comprise it. This can result in a perceived abundance of insightful ideas and conclusions with powerful themes pertaining to what is often described as "the bigger picture". These ideas generally involve (but are not limited to) insight into philosophy, science, spirituality, society, culture, universal progress, humanity, loved ones, the finite nature of our lives, history, the present moment, and future possibilities.

There have been models linking multidimensional personality traits to cognitive performance.[4] Chronic psychedelic use influences the user's personality and experienced users score significantly better than controls on several neuropsychological tests.[5]

Analysis enhancement is often accompanied by other coinciding effects such as stimulation, personal bias suppression, conceptual thinking, and thought connectivity. It is most commonly induced under the influence of moderate dosages of stimulant and nootropic compounds, such as amphetamine, methylphenidate, nicotine, and caffeine.[1][3] However, it can also occur in a more powerful although less consistent form under the influence of psychedelics such as certain LSD, psilocybin, and mescaline.[5]

Anxiety

Main article: Anxiety

Anxiety is the experience of negative feelings of apprehension, worry, and general unease.[6] These feelings can range from subtle and ignorable to intense and overwhelming enough to trigger panic attacks or feelings of impending doom. Anxiety is often accompanied by nervous behaviour such as restlessness, difficulty concentrating, irritability, and muscular tension.[7]

Fear is the emotional response to real or perceived imminent threat, whereas anxiety is anticipation of future threat. Obviously, these two states overlap, but they also differ, with fear more often associated with surges of autonomic arousal necessary for fight or flight, thoughts of immediate danger, and escape behaviors, and anxiety more often associated with muscle tension and vigilance in preparation for future danger and cautious or avoidant behaviors.[7][8]

Psychoactive substance-induced anxiety can be caused as an inescapable effect of the drug itself,[7] by a lack of experience with the substance or its intensity, as an enhancement of a pre-existing state of mind, or by the experience of negative hallucinations.

Anxiety is often accompanied by other coinciding effects such as depression and irritability. It is most commonly induced under the influence of moderate dosages of hallucinogenic compounds, such as cannabinoids,[9] psychedelics,[10] dissociatives, and deliriants.[11] However, it can also occur during the withdrawal symptoms of GABAergic depressants[12] and during stimulant comedowns.[13]

Creativity enhancement

Creativity enhancement is a perceived increase in one's capability to imagine new ideas, create art, or think about existing concepts in a novel manner.[14][15][16] This effect is particularly useful to artists of any sort as it can help a person overcome creative blocks on existing projects and induce inspiration for entirely new projects. Creativity enhancement can make imaginative activities more enjoyable and effortless in the moment and the inspiration from it can benefit the individual even after the effect has worn off.

A well-known example of psychedelic creativity enhancement comes from the Nobel Prize winning chemist Dr. Kary Mullis, who invented a method for copying DNA segments known as the PCR and is quoted as saying: "Would I have invented PCR if I hadn't taken LSD? I seriously doubt it. I could sit on a DNA molecule and watch the polymers go by. I learned that partly on psychedelic drugs".[17] In addition, although dubious, it has been claimed Francis Crick experimented with LSD during the time he helped elucidate the structure of DNA.[18]

Creativity enhancement is often accompanied by other coinciding effects such as thought connectivity, motivation enhancement, personal bias suppression, analysis enhancement, and thought acceleration in a manner which further amplifies a person's creativity. It is most commonly induced under the influence of moderate dosages of psychedelic compounds, such as LSD, psilocybin, and mescaline.[19][20][21] However, it can also occur to a lesser extent under the influence of cannabinoids,[22][23] dissociatives,[24] and stimulants.

Dream potentiation

Main article: Dream potentiation

Dream potentiation is an effect which increases the subjective intensity, vividness, and frequency of sleeping dream states.[25][26] This effect also results in dreams having a more complex and incohesive plot with a higher level of detail and definition.[26] Additionally, the effect causes a greatly increased likelihood of them becoming lucid dreams.

Dream potentiation is most commonly induced under the influence of moderate dosages of oneirogenic compounds, a class of hallucinogen that is used to specifically potentiate dreams when taken before sleep.[27] However, it can also occur as a residual side effect from falling asleep under the influence of an extremely wide variety of substances. At other times, it can occur as a relatively persistent effect that has arisen as a symptom of hallucinogen persisting perception disorder (HPPD).

Ego inflation

Main article: Ego inflation

Ego inflation is an effect that magnifies and enhances one's own ego and self-regard in a manner which results in feeling an increased sense of confidence, superiority, and general arrogance.[28] During this state, it can often feel that one is considerably more intelligent, important, and capable in comparison to those around them. This occurs in a manner which is similar to the psychological condition known as narcissistic personality disorder.[29]

At lower levels, this experience can result in an enhanced ability to handle social situations due to a heightened sense of confidence.[30] However, at higher levels, it can result in a reduced ability to handle social situations due to magnifying egoistic behavioural traits that may come across as distinctly obnoxious, narcissistic, and selfish to other people.

It is worth noting that regular and repeated long-term exposure to this effect can leave certain individuals with persistent behavioural traits of ego inflation, even when sober, within their day to day life.

Ego inflation is often accompanied by other coinciding effects such as disinhibition, irritability, and paranoia in a manner which can lead to destructive behaviors and violent tendencies.[30] It is most commonly induced under the influence of moderate dosages of stimulant compounds, particularly dopaminergic stimulants such as methamphetamine and cocaine.[28][30][31][32] However, it may also occur under the influence of other compounds such as GABAergic depressants[28] and certain dissociatives.

Emotion enhancement

Main article: Emotion enhancement

Emotion enhancement is an effect which greatly amplifies and enhances a person's current emotional state beyond normal levels of intensity.[33][34][35]

Unlike many other subjective effects such as euphoria or anxiety this effect does not actively induce specific emotions regardless of a person's current state of mind and mental stability. Instead, it works by passively amplifying and enhancing the genuine emotions that a person is already feeling prior to ingesting the drug or prior to the onset of this effect. This causes emotion enhancement to be equally capable of manifesting in both a positive and negative direction.[33][35][36]

For example, an individual who is currently feeling somewhat anxious or emotionally unstable may become overwhelmed with intensified negative emotions, paranoia, and confusion. In contrast, an individual who is generally feeling positive and emotionally stable is more likely to find themselves overwhelmed with states of emotional euphoria, happiness, and feelings of general contentment. The intensity of emotional states felt under emotion enhancement can shape the tone of a trip and predispose the user to other effects, such as mania or unity in positive states and thought loops or feelings of impending doom in negative states.[35]

Emotion enhancement is most commonly induced under the influence of moderate dosages of psychedelic compounds, such as LSD, psilocybin, and mescaline.[33][34][35] However, it can also occur under the influence of cannabinoids, GABAergic depressants,[37][38] and stimulants.[36][39]

Empathy, affection, and sociability enhancement

Empathy, affection, and sociability enhancement is the experience of a mind state which is dominated by intense feelings of compassion, talkativeness, and happiness.[40][41][42] The experience of this effect creates a wide range of subjective changes to a person's perception of their feelings towards other people and themselves. These are described and documented in the list below:

  • Increased sociability and the feeling that communication comes easier and more naturally.
  • Increased urge to communicate or express one's affectionate feelings towards others, even if they happen to be strangers.
  • Increased feelings of empathy, love, and connection with others.
  • Increased motivation to resolve social conflicts and improve interpersonal relationships.
  • Decreased negative emotions and mental states such as stress, anxiety, and fear.
  • Decreased insecurity, defensiveness, and fear of emotional injury or rejection from others.
  • Decreased irritability, aggression, anger, and jealousy.

Empathy, affection, and sociability enhancement is often accompanied by other coinciding effects such as stimulation, personal bias suppression, motivation enhancement, and anxiety suppression. It is most commonly induced under the influence of moderate dosages of entactogenic compounds such as MDMA,[43][44] 4-FA,[45][46] and 2C-B.[47][48] However, it can also subtly occur to a much lesser extent under the influence of GABAergic depressants, and certain stimulants.[49]

Focus enhancement

Main article: Focus enhancement

Focus enhancement is the experience of an increased ability to selectively concentrate on an aspect of the environment while ignoring other things. It can be best characterized by feelings of intense concentration which can allow one to continuously focus on and perform tasks which would otherwise be considered too monotonous, boring, or dull to not get distracted from.[50][51]

The degree of focus induced by this effect can be much stronger than what a person is capable of sober. It can allow for hours of effortless, single-minded, and continuous focus on a particular activity to the exclusion of all other considerations such as eating and attending to bodily functions. However, although focus enhancement can improve a person’s ability to engage in tasks and use time effectively, it is worth noting that it can also cause a person to focus intensely and spend excess time on unimportant activities.

Focus enhancement is often accompanied by other coinciding effects such as motivation enhancement, thought acceleration, and stimulation. It is most commonly induced under the influence of moderate dosages of stimulant and nootropic compounds such as amphetamine,[52] methylphenidate,[53] modafinil,[54] and caffeine.[55][56] However, it is worth noting that the same compounds which induce this mind state at moderate dosages will also often result in the opposite effect of focus suppression at heavier dosages.[57]

Immersion enhancement

Main article: Immersion enhancement

Immersion enhancement is an effect which results in a pronounced increase in one's tendency to become fully captivated and engrossed by external stimuli such as film, TV shows, video games, and various other forms of media.[58][59][60][61] This greatly increases one's suspension of disbelief, increases one’s empathy with the characters, suppresses one's memory of the "outside world", and allows one to become engaged on a level that is largely unattainable during everyday sober living.

At its highest point of intensity, immersion enhancement can reach a level in which the person begins to truly believe that the media they are consuming is a real-life event that is actually happening in front of them or is being relayed through a screen. This is likely a result of the effect synergizing with other accompanying components such as internal or external hallucinations, delusions, memory suppression, and suggestibility enhancement. Immersion enhancement often exaggerates the emotional response a person has towards media they are engaged with. Whether or not this experience is enjoyable can differ drastically depending on various factors such as the emotional tone and familiarity of what is being perceived.

Immersion enhancement is most commonly induced under the influence of moderate dosages of dissociative compounds, such as ketamine, PCP, and DXM. However, it can also occur to a lesser extent under the influence of psychedelics[61] and cannabinoids.

Increased music appreciation

Increased music appreciation is a general sense of an increased enjoyment of music. When music is listened to during this state, not only does it subjectively sound better, but the perceived music and lyrical content may have a profound impact on the listener.[33][62][63][64][65]

This experience can give one a sense of hyper-awareness of every sound, lyric, melody, and complex layer of noise within a song in addition to an enhanced ability to individually comprehend their significance and interplay. The perceived emotional intent of the musician and the meaning of the music may also be felt in a greater level clarity than that which is attainable during everyday sober living.[63] This effect can result in the belief, legitimate or delusional, that one has connected with the “true meaning” or “spirit” behind an artist’s song. During particularly enjoyable songs, this effect can result in feelings of overwhelming harmony[65] and a general sense of appreciation that can leave the person with a deep sense of connection towards the artist they are listening to.

Increased music appreciation is commonly mistaken as a purely auditory effect but is more likely the result of several coinciding components such as novelty enhancement, personal meaning enhancement, emotionality enhancement, and auditory enhancement. It is most commonly induced under the influence of moderate dosages of hallucinogenic compounds, such as psychedelics,[33][64][66] dissociatives,[67] and cannabinoids.[63] However, it can also occur to a lesser extent under the influence of stimulants[63][66] and GABAergic depressants.

Increased sense of humor

Increased sense of humor is as a general enhancement of the likelihood and degree to which a person finds stimuli to be humorous and amusing. During this state, a person's sensitivity to finding things funny is noticeably amplified, often to the point that they will begin uncontrollably laughing at trivial things without any intelligible reason or apparent cause.

In group settings, the experience of witnessing another person who is laughing intensely for no apparent reason can itself become a contagious trigger which induces semi-uncontrollable laughter within the people around them. In extreme cases, this can often form a lengthy feedback loop in which people begin to laugh hysterically at the absurdity of not being able to stop laughing and not knowing what started the laughter to begin with.

Increased sense of humor is often accompanied by other coinciding effects such as emotion enhancement and novelty enhancement. It is most commonly induced under the influence of moderate dosages of certain hallucinogenic compounds, such as psychedelics, cannabinoids, and mescaline. However, it can also occur to a much lesser extent under the influence of dissociatives, GABAergic depressants, and stimulants.

Irritability

Main article: Irritability

Irritability is the experience of heightened feelings of stress, impatience, annoyance, and anger.[68]

This effect, especially when strong, can sometimes cause violent or aggressive outbursts in a small subset of people who may be predisposed to it. The chances of somebody responding in such a way differs wildly between people and depends on how susceptible an individual is to irritability and how well they cope with it. It is also worth noting that this typically only affects those who were already susceptible to aggressive behaviours. However, regardless of the person, this effect results in a lower ability to tolerate frustrations, negative stimuli, and other people. A person undergoing this effect may be prone to lashing out at others, fits of anger, or other behaviours that would be uncharacteristic for them sober.

Irritability is often accompanied by other coinciding effects such as anxiety, paranoia, and ego inflation. It is most commonly induced during the after effects of heavy dosages of stimulant compounds, such as cocaine,[69] methamphetamine,[70] and methylphenidate.[71] However, it can be a withdrawal symptom of almost any substance, and can to a lesser extent present itself during alcohol intoxication.[72]

Memory enhancement

Main article: Memory enhancement

Memory enhancement is an improvement in a person's ability to recall or retain memories.[73][74][75][76] The experience of this effect can make it easier for a person to access and remember past memories at a greater level of detail when compared to that of everyday sober living. It can also help one retain new information that may then be more easily recalled once the person is no longer under the influence of the psychoactive substance.

Memory enhancement is often accompanied by other coinciding effects such as analysis enhancement and thought acceleration. It is most commonly induced under the influence of moderate dosages of stimulant and nootropic compounds, such as methylphenidate,[77] caffeine,[75] Noopept,[78] nicotine,[79] and modafinil.[80]

Motivation enhancement

Motivation enhancement is an increased desire to perform tasks and accomplish goals in a productive manner.[81][82][83] This includes tasks and goals that would normally be considered too monotonous or overwhelming to fully commit oneself to.

Motivation enhancement is often accompanied by other coinciding effects such as stimulation and thought acceleration in a manner which further increases one's productivity. It is most commonly induced under the influence of moderate dosages of stimulant and nootropic compounds, such as amphetamine,[84] methylphenidate,[82] Noopept, and modafinil. However, it may also occur to a much lesser extent under the influence of certain opioids, and GABAergic depressants.

Novelty enhancement

Main article: Novelty enhancement

Novelty enhancement is a feeling of increased fascination and appreciation attributed to specific parts or the entirety of one's external environment. This can result in an often overwhelming impression that everyday concepts such as nature, existence, common events, and even household objects are now considerably more profound, interesting, and important.

The experience of this effect commonly forces those who undergo it to acknowledge, consider, and appreciate the things around them in a level of detail and intensity which remains largely unparalleled throughout every day sobriety. It is often generally described using phrases such as "a child-like sense of wonder" or "seeing the world through fresh eyes".

Novelty enhancement is often accompanied by other coinciding effects such as personal bias suppression and emotion enhancement in a manner which further intensifies the experience. It is most commonly induced under the influence of moderate dosages of psychedelic compounds, such as LSD, psilocybin, and mescaline. However, it can also occur to a lesser extent under the influence of cannabinoids, dissociatives, and entactogens.

Personal meaning enhancement

Personal meaning enhancement is the experience of a considerably increased sense of personal significance becoming attributed to external stimuli, innocuous situations, and coincidences. For example, one may feel that the lyrics of a song or events in a film directly relate to their life in a meaningful and distinct manner that is not usually felt during everyday sobriety. This feeling can continue to occur even when it is rationally understood that the external stimuli does not genuinely relate to the person experiencing it in such a direct manner.

At its highest level, this effect will often synergize with delusions in a manner which can result in one genuinely believing that innocuous events are directly related to them. For example, one may begin to believe that the plot of a film is about their life or that a song was written for them. This phenomenon is well established within psychology and is commonly known as a "delusion of reference."[85][86]

Suggestibility enhancement

Suggestibility enhancement is an increased tendency to accept and act on the suggestions of others.[87][88] A common example of suggestibility enhancement in action would be a trip sitter deliberately making a person believe a false statement without question simply by telling it to them as true, even if the statement would usually be easily recognizable as impossible or absurd. If this is successfully accomplished, it can potentially result in the experience of relevant accompanying hallucinations and delusions which further solidify the belief which has been suggested to them.

Suggestibility enhancement most commonly occurs under the influence of heavy dosages of hallucinogenic compounds, such as psychedelics, dissociatives, deliriants, and cannabinoids. This holds particularly true for users who are inexperienced or currently undergoing delusions and memory suppression. It's worth noting that this effect has been studied extensively by the scientific literature and has a relatively large body of data confirming its presence across multiple hallucinogens. These include LSD[89], mescaline[87], psilocybin,[87] cannabis[90], ketamine[91], and nitrous oxide[92]. However, anecdotal reports suggest that it may also occur to a lesser extent under the influence of GABAergic depressants such as alcohol and benzodiazepines.

Thought acceleration

Main article: Thought acceleration

Thought acceleration (also known as racing thoughts)[93] is the experience of thought processes being sped up significantly in comparison to that of everyday sobriety.[94][95] When experiencing this effect, it will often feel as if one rapid-fire thought after the other is being generated in incredibly quick succession. Thoughts while undergoing this effect are not necessarily qualitatively different, but greater in their volume and speed. However, they are commonly associated with a change in mood that can be either positive or negative.[96][97]

Thought acceleration is often accompanied by other coinciding effects such as stimulation, anxiety, and analysis enhancement in a manner which not only increases the speed of thought, but also signifigantly enhances the sharpness of a person's mental clarity. It is most commonly induced under the influence of moderate dosages of stimulant and nootropic compounds, such as amphetamine, methylphenidate, modafinil, and MDMA. However, it can also occur under the influence of certain stimulating psychedelics such as LSD, 2C-E, DOC, AMT.

Thought connectivity

Main article: Thought connectivity

Thought connectivity is an alteration of a person's thought stream which is characterized by a distinct increase in wandering thoughts which connect into each other through a fluid association of ideas. During this state, thoughts may be subjectively experienced as a continuous stream of vaguely related ideas which tenuously connect into each other by incorporating a concept that was contained within the previous thought. When experienced, it is often likened to a complex game of word association.

During this state, it is often difficult for the person to consciously guide the direction of their thoughts in a manner that leads into a state of increased distractibility. This will usually also result in one's train of thought contemplating an extremely broad variety of subjects, which can range from important, trivial, insightful, and nonsensical topics.

Thought connectivity is often accompanied by other coinciding effects such as thought acceleration and creativity enhancement. It is most commonly induced under the influence of moderate dosages of psychedelic compounds, such as LSD, psilocybin, and mescaline. However, it can also occur to a lesser extent under the influence of dissociatives, stimulants, and cannabinoids.

Thought organization

Main article: Thought organization

Thought organization is as a state of mind in which one's ability to analyze and categorize conceptual information using a systematic and logical thought process is considerably increased. It seemingly occurs through reducing thoughts which are unrelated or irrelevant to the topic at hand, therefore improving one's capacity for a structured and cohesive thought stream. This effect also seems to allow the person to hold a greater amount of relevant information in their train of thought which can be useful for extended mental calculations, articulating ideas, and analyzing logical arguments.

Thought organization is often accompanied by other coinciding effects such as analysis enhancement and thought connectivity. It is most commonly induced under the influence of mild dosages of stimulant and nootropic compounds, such as modafinil, Noopept, and theanine. However, this effect can occur to a lesser extent under the influence of certain cannabis strains and spontaneously during psychedelic states. It is also worth noting that the same compounds which induce this mind state at light to moderate dosages can often result in the opposite effect of thought disorganization at heavier dosages.

Wakefulness

Main article: Wakefulness

Wakefulness is an increased ability to stay conscious without feeling sleepy combined with a decreased need to sleep.[98] It is contrasted with stimulation in that it does not directly increase one's energy levels above a normal baseline but instead produces feelings of a wakeful, well-rested, and alert state. If one is sleepy before using this substance, the impulse to sleep will fade, keeping one’s eyes open will become easier, and the cognitive fog of exhaustion will be reduced.[99] However, sufficiently accumulated sleep deficiency can overpower or negate this effect in extreme cases.

Wakefulness is most commonly induced under the influence of moderate dosages of a wide variety of compounds such as stimulants, nootropics, and psychedelics. However, it is worth noting that the few compounds which selectively induce this effect without a number of other accompanying effects are referred to as eugeroics or wakefulness-promoting agents. These include modafinil,[100] armodafinil,[101] and Noopept.

See also

References

  1. 1.0 1.1 Fillmore, M. T., Kelly, T. H., & Martin, C. A. (2005). Effects of d-amphetamine in human models of information processing and inhibitory control. Drug and alcohol dependence, 77(2), 151-159. https://dx.doi.org/10.1016%2Fj.drugalcdep.2004.07.013
  2. Bättig, K., & Buzzi, R. (1986). Effect of coffee on the speed of subject-paced information processing. Neuropsychobiology, 16(2-3), 126-130. https://doi.org/10.1159/000118312
  3. 3.0 3.1 Warburton, D. M., Bersellini, E., & Sweeney, E. (2001). An evaluation of a caffeinated taurine drink on mood, memory and information processing in healthy volunteers without caffeine abstinence. Psychopharmacology, 158(3), 322-328. https://doi.org/10.1007/s002130100884
  4. Humphreys, M. S., & Revelle, W. (1984). Personality, motivation, and performance: a theory of the relationship between individual differences and information processing. Psychological review, 91(2), 153. http://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/0033-295X.91.2.153
  5. 5.0 5.1 Bouso, J. C., Palhano-Fontes, F., Rodríguez-Fornells, A., Ribeiro, S., Sanches, R., Crippa, J. A. S., ... & Riba, J. (2015). Long-term use of psychedelic drugs is associated with differences in brain structure and personality in humans. European Neuropsychopharmacology, 25(4), 483-492. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.euroneuro.2015.01.008
  6. Anxiety definition (American Psychology Association) | http://www.apa.org/topics/anxiety/
  7. 7.0 7.1 7.2 American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.), 189-190. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596.dsm05
  8. Barkus, C., McHugh, S. B., Sprengel, R., Seeburg, P. H., Rawlins, J. N. P., & Bannerman, D. M. (2010). Hippocampal NMDA receptors and anxiety: at the interface between cognition and emotion. European journal of pharmacology, 626(1), 49-56. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ejphar.2009.10.014
  9. Crippa, J. A., Zuardi, A. W., Martín‐Santos, R., Bhattacharyya, S., Atakan, Z., McGuire, P., & Fusar‐Poli, P. (2009). Cannabis and anxiety: a critical review of the evidence. Human Psychopharmacology: Clinical and Experimental, 24(7), 515-523. https://doi.org/10.1002/hup.1048
  10. Wolbach, A. B., Miner, E. J., & Isbell, H. (1962). Comparison of psilocin with psilocybin, mescaline and LSD-25. Psychopharmacology, 3(3), 219-223. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00412109
  11. Datura effects (Erowid) | https://erowid.org/plants/datura/datura_effects.shtml
  12. Fontaine, R., Chouinard, G., & Annable, L. (1984). Rebound anxiety in anxious patients after abrupt withdrawal of benzodiazepine treatment. Am J Psychiatry, 141(7), 848-852. https://doi.org/10.1176/ajp.141.7.848
  13. Williamson, S., Gossop, M., Powis, B., Griffiths, P., Fountain, J., & Strang, J. (1997). Adverse effects of stimulant drugs in a community sample of drug users. Drug and alcohol dependence, 44(2-3), 87-94. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0376-8716(96)01324-5
  14. Iszáj, Fruzsina; Griffiths, Mark D.; Demetrovics, Zsolt (2016). "Creativity and Psychoactive Substance Use: A Systematic Review". International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction. 15 (5): 1135–1149. doi:10.1007/s11469-016-9709-8. ISSN 1557-1874. 
  15. What exactly is creativity? (American Psychological Association) | http://www.apa.org/monitor/nov03/creativity.aspx
  16. Can psychedelic drugs increase creativity? (MAPS) | https://www.maps.org/news/multimedia-library/3171-can-psychedelic-drugs-enhance-creativity
  17. Hongbao, Ma. "Development application of polymerase chain reaction (PCR)." J. Am. Sci 1.3 (2005): 4-5. | http://www.sciencepub.net/american/0103/01-0198-%20mahongbao-am.pdf
  18. Brooks, M. (2012). Free radicals: The secret anarchy of science. The Overlook Press. https://books.google.com/books?id=nDMjCQAAQBAJ&lpg=PT142&ots=-yvt2yhPqk&dq=He%20listened%20with%20rapt%2C%20amused%20attention%20to%20what%20I%20told%20him%20about%20the%20role%20of%20LSD%20in%20his%20Nobel%20Prize-winning%20discovery.%20He%20gave%20no%20intimation%20of%20surprise.%20When%20I%20had%20finished%2C%20he%20said%3A%20'Print%20a%20word%20of%20it%20and%20I'll%20sue.'&pg=PT142#v=onepage&q=He%20listened%20with%20rapt,%20amused%20attention%20to%20what%20I%20told%20him%20about%20the%20role%20of%20LSD%20in%20his%20Nobel%20Prize-winning%20discovery.%20He%20gave%20no%20intimation%20of%20surprise.%20When%20I%20had%20finished,%20he%20said:%20'Print%20a%20word%20of%20it%20and%20I'll%20sue&f=false
  19. Sessa, B. (2008). "Is it time to revisit the role of psychedelic drugs in enhancing human creativity?". Journal of Psychopharmacology. 22 (8): 821–827. doi:10.1177/0269881108091597. ISSN 0269-8811. 
  20. Krippiwr, Stanley (2008). "Research in creativity and psychedelic drugs". International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis. 25 (4): 274–290. doi:10.1080/00207147708415985. ISSN 0020-7144. 
  21. Krippner, Stanley (1985). "Psychedelic Drugs and Creativity". Journal of Psychoactive Drugs. 17 (4): 235–246. doi:10.1080/02791072.1985.10524328. ISSN 0279-1072. 
  22. Green, Bob; Kavanagh, David; Young, Ross (2003). "Being stoned: a review of self-reported cannabis effects". Drug and Alcohol Review. 22 (4): 453–460. doi:10.1080/09595230310001613976. ISSN 0959-5236. 
  23. Kowal, Mikael A.; Hazekamp, Arno; Colzato, Lorenza S.; van Steenbergen, Henk; van der Wee, Nic J. A.; Durieux, Jeffrey; Manai, Meriem; Hommel, Bernhard (2014). "Cannabis and creativity: highly potent cannabis impairs divergent thinking in regular cannabis users". Psychopharmacology. 232 (6): 1123–1134. doi:10.1007/s00213-014-3749-1. ISSN 0033-3158. 
  24. Journey through the K-hole: Phenomenological aspects of ketamine use | https://www.drugandalcoholdependence.com/article/S0376-8716(08)00055-0/abstract
  25. Pavel, S., Goldstein, R., & Petrescu, M. (1980). Vasotocin, melatonin and narcolepsy: possible involvement of the pineal gland in its patho-physiological mechanism. Peptides, 1(4), 281-284. https://doi.org/10.1016/0196-9781(80)90003-0
  26. 26.0 26.1 Pace‐Schott, E. F., Gersh, T., Silvestri, R., Stickgold, R., Salzman, C., & Hobson, J. A. (2001). SSRI treatment suppresses dream recall frequency but increases subjective dream intensity in normal subjects. Journal of Sleep Research, 10(2), 129-142. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1365-2869.2001.00249.x
  27. Oneirogens | http://oneironauticum.com/oneirogens/
  28. 28.0 28.1 28.2 Nour, M. M., Evans, L., Nutt, D., & Carhart-Harris, R. L. (2016). Ego-dissolution and psychedelics: validation of the ego-dissolution inventory (EDI). Frontiers in human neuroscience, 10, 269. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2016.00269
  29. American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.), 669-672. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596.dsm18
  30. 30.0 30.1 30.2 Spotts, J. V., & Shontz, F. C. (1984). Drug-induced ego states. I. Cocaine: phenomenology and implications. International Journal of the Addictions, 19(2), 119-151. https://doi.org/10.3109/10826088409057173
  31. Woodham, R. L. (1988). A self-psychological consideration in cocaine addiction. Alcoholism Treatment Quarterly, 4(3), 41-46. https://doi.org/10.1300/J020V04N03_04
  32. Kuypers, K. P. C., Steenbergen, L., Theunissen, E. L., Toennes, S. W., & Ramaekers, J. G. (2015). Emotion recognition during cocaine intoxication. European Neuropsychopharmacology, 25(11), 1914-1921. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.euroneuro.2015.08.012
  33. 33.0 33.1 33.2 33.3 33.4 Kaelen, M., Barrett, F. S., Roseman, L., Lorenz, R., Family, N., Bolstridge, M., ... & Carhart-Harris, R. L. (2015). LSD enhances the emotional response to music. Psychopharmacology, 232(19), 3607-3614. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-015-4014-y
  34. 34.0 34.1 Hartogsohn, Ido. 2018. “The Meaning-Enhancing Properties of Psychedelics and Their Mediator Role in Psychedelic Therapy, Spirituality, and Creativity.” Frontiers in Neuroscience 12 (1): 129. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2018.00129
  35. 35.0 35.1 35.2 35.3 Swanson, L. R. (2018). Unifying Theories of Psychedelic Drug Effects. Frontiers in pharmacology, 9, 172. https://dx.doi.org/10.3389%2Ffphar.2018.00172
  36. 36.0 36.1 Miller, M. A., Bershad, A. K., & de Wit, H. (2015). Drug effects on responses to emotional facial expressions: recent findings. Behavioural pharmacology, 26(6), 571. https://dx.doi.org/10.1097%2FFBP.0000000000000164
  37. Kamboj, S. K., Joye, A., Bisby, J. A., Das, R. K., Platt, B., & Curran, H. V. (2013). Processing of facial affect in social drinkers: a dose–response study of alcohol using dynamic emotion expressions. Psychopharmacology, 227(1), 31-39. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-012-2940-5
  38. Philippot, P., Kornreich, C., Blairy, S., Baert, I., Dulk, A. D., Bon, O. L., ... & Verbanck, P. (1999). Alcoholics’ deficits in the decoding of emotional facial expression. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 23(6), 1031-1038. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1530-0277.1999.tb04221.x
  39. Wardle, M. C., Garner, M. J., Munafò, M. R., & de Wit, H. (2012). Amphetamine as a social drug: effects of d-amphetamine on social processing and behavior. Psychopharmacology, 223(2), 199-210. https://dx.doi.org/10.1007%2Fs00213-012-2708-y
  40. Nichols, D. E. (1986). Differences between the mechanism of action of MDMA, MBDB, and the classic hallucinogens. Identification of a new therapeutic class: entactogens. Journal of psychoactive drugs, 18(4), 305-313. https://doi.org/10.1080/02791072.1986.10472362
  41. The Great Entactogen - Empathogen Debate (from the Newsletter of the Multidisciplinary Association for Psychedelic Studies, MAPS - Volume 4 Number 2 Summer 1993 - pp 47-49) | https://www.maps.org/news-letters/v04n2/04247eed.html
  42. Bedi, G., Hyman, D., & de Wit, H. (2010). Is ecstasy an “empathogen”? Effects of±3, 4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine on prosocial feelings and identification of emotional states in others. Biological psychiatry, 68(12), 1134-1140. https://dx.doi.org/10.1016%2Fj.biopsych.2010.08.003
  43. Scahill, L., & Anderson, G. M. (2010). Is ecstasy an empathogen?. Biological psychiatry, 68(12), 1082. https://dx.doi.org/10.1016%2Fj.biopsych.2010.10.020
  44. MDMA effects (Erowid) | https://erowid.org/chemicals/mdma/mdma_effects.shtml
  45. 4-FA (The Drug Classroom) | https://thedrugclassroom.com/video/4-fluoroamphetamine-4-fa/
  46. 4-Fluoroamphetamine(4-FA) – Critical Review Report | http://www.who.int/medicines/access/controlled-substances/5.4_4-FA_CRev.pdf?ua=1
  47. 2C-B effects (Erowid) | https://erowid.org/chemicals/2cb/2cb_effects.shtml
  48. González, D., Torrens, M., & Farré, M. (2015). Acute effects of the novel psychoactive drug 2C-B on emotions. BioMed research international, 2015. https://dx.doi.org/10.1155%2F2015%2F643878
  49. Wardle, M. C., Garner, M. J., Munafò, M. R., & de Wit, H. (2012). Amphetamine as a social drug: effects of d-amphetamine on social processing and behavior. Psychopharmacology, 223(2), 199-210. https://dx.doi.org/10.1007%2Fs00213-012-2708-y
  50. Ford, J. A., & Schroeder, R. D. (2008). Academic strain and non-medical use of prescription stimulants among college students. Deviant Behavior, 30(1), 26-53. https://doi.org/10.1080/01639620802049900
  51. Riccio, C. A., Waldrop, J. J., Reynolds, C. R., & Lowe, P. (2001). Effects of stimulants on the continuous performance test (CPT) implications for CPT use and interpretation. The Journal of neuropsychiatry and clinical neurosciences, 13(3), 326-335. https://doi.org/10.1176/jnp.13.3.326
  52. Seiden, L. S., Sabol, K. E., & Ricaurte, G. A. (1993). Amphetamine: effects on catecholamine systems and behavior. Annual review of pharmacology and toxicology, 33(1), 639-676. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.pa.33.040193.003231
  53. Sprague, R. L., & Sleator, E. K. (1977). Methylphenidate in hyperkinetic children: Differences in dose effects on learning and social behavior. Science, 198(4323), 1274-1276. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.337493
  54. Randall, D. C., Viswanath, A., Bharania, P., Elsabagh, S. M., Hartley, D. E., Shneerson, J. M., & File, S. E. (2005). Does modafinil enhance cognitive performance in young volunteers who are not sleep-deprived?. Journal of Clinical Psychopharmacology, 25(2), 175-179. https://doi.org/10.1097/01.jcp.0000155816.21467.25
  55. Bernstein, G. A., Carroll, M. E., Crosby, R. D., Perwien, A. R., Go, F. S., & Benowitz, N. L. (1994). Caffeine effects on learning, performance, and anxiety in normal school-age children. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 33(3), 407-415. https://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-199403000-00016
  56. Caffeine effects (Erowid) | https://erowid.org/chemicals/caffeine/caffeine_effects.shtml
  57. Salo, R., Nordahl, T. E., Natsuaki, Y., Leamon, M. H., Galloway, G. P., Waters, C., ... & Buonocore, M. H. (2007). Attentional control and brain metabolite levels in methamphetamine abusers. Biological psychiatry, 61(11), 1272-1280. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.07.031
  58. Waller, N., Putnam, F. W., & Carlson, E. B. (1996). Types of dissociation and dissociative types: A taxometric analysis of dissociative experiences. Psychological methods, 1(3), 300. https://doi.org/10.1037/1082-989X.1.3.300
  59. Giesbrecht, T., Merckelbach, H., & Geraerts, E. (2007). The dissociative experiences taxon is related to fantasy proneness. The Journal of nervous and mental disease, 195(9), 769-772. https://doi.org/10.1097/NMD.0b013e318142ce55
  60. Levin, R., & Spei, E. (2004). Relationship of purported measures of pathological and nonpathological dissociation to self-reported psychological distress and fantasy immersion. Assessment, 11(2), 160-168. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191103256377
  61. 61.0 61.1 Lynn, C. D. (2005). Adaptive and maladaptive dissociation: An epidemiological and anthropological comparison and proposition for an expanded dissociation model. Anthropology of Consciousness, 16(2), 16-49. https://doi.org/10.1525/ac.2005.16.2.16
  62. Kaelen, M., Roseman, L., Kahan, J., Santos-Ribeiro, A., Orban, C., Lorenz, R., ... & Wall, M. B. (2016). LSD modulates music-induced imagery via changes in parahippocampal connectivity. European Neuropsychopharmacology, 26(7), 1099-1109. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.euroneuro.2016.03.018
  63. 63.0 63.1 63.2 63.3 Fachner, J. (2002). The space between the notes-Research on cannabis and music perception. Looking Back, Looking Ahead-Popular Music Studies, 20, 308-319. http://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/viewdoc/download?doi=10.1.1.467.6346&rep=rep1&type=pdf
  64. 64.0 64.1 Nichols, D. E. (2016). Psychedelics. Pharmacological reviews, 68(2), 264-355. https://dx.doi.org/10.1124%2Fpr.115.011478
  65. 65.0 65.1 Kaelen, M., Giribaldi, B., Raine, J., Evans, L., Timmerman, C., Rodriguez, N., ... & Carhart-Harris, R. (2018). The hidden therapist: evidence for a central role of music in psychedelic therapy. Psychopharmacology, 235(2), 505-519. https://dx.doi.org/10.1007%2Fs00213-017-4820-5
  66. 66.0 66.1 McGlothlin, W., Cohen, S., & McGlothlin, M. S. (1967). Long lasting effects of LSD on normals. Archives of General Psychiatry, 17(5), 521-532. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.1967.01730290009002
  67. Lim, D. K. (2003). Ketamine associated psychedelic effects and dependence. Singapore Med J, 44(1), 31-34. https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Dominic_Lim2/publication/10746289_Ketamine_associated_psychedelic_effects_and_dependence/links/54436e1e0cf2a76a3ccb1707.pdf
  68. https://psychologydictionary.org/irritability/
  69. Cocaine effects (Erowid) | https://erowid.org/chemicals/cocaine/cocaine_effects.shtml
  70. Sommers, I., Baskin, D., & Baskin-Sommers, A. (2006). Methamphetamine use among young adults: health and social consequences. Addictive behaviors, 31(8), 1469-1476. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2005.10.004
  71. Ahmann, P. A., Waltonen, S. J., Theye, F. W., Olson, K. A., & Van Erem, A. J. (1993). Placebo-controlled evaluation of Ritalin side effects. Pediatrics, 91(6), 1101-1106. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/8502509
  72. People Who Suppress Anger Become Violent When Drunk (livescience.com) | https://www.livescience.com/6640-people-suppress-anger-violent-drunk.html
  73. Mondadori, C., Möbius, H. J., & Borkowski, J. (1996). The GABAB receptor antagonist CGP 36 742 and the nootropic oxiracetam facilitate the formation of long-term memory. Behavioural brain research, 77(1-2), 223-225. https://doi.org/10.1016/0166-4328(95)00222-7
  74. Ilieva, I. P., Hook, C. J., & Farah, M. J. (2015). Prescription stimulants' effects on healthy inhibitory control, working memory, and episodic memory: a meta-analysis. Journal of cognitive neuroscience, 27(6), 1069-1089. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00776
  75. 75.0 75.1 Borota, D., Murray, E., Keceli, G., Chang, A., Watabe, J. M., Ly, M., ... & Yassa, M. A. (2014). Post-study caffeine administration enhances memory consolidation in humans. Nature neuroscience, 17(2), 201. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3623
  76. Morgan, A., & Stevens, J. (2010). Does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons? Results of a randomized, placebo-controlled, double-blind trial. The Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine, 16(7), 753-759. https://doi.org/10.1089/acm.2009.0342
  77. Mehta, M. A., Owen, A. M., Sahakian, B. J., Mavaddat, N., Pickard, J. D., & Robbins, T. W. (2000). Methylphenidate enhances working memory by modulating discrete frontal and parietal lobe regions in the human brain. Journal of Neuroscience, 20(6), RC65-RC65. http://www.adders.org/camb%20research.pdf
  78. Ostrovskaia, R. U., Gudasheva, T. A., Voronina, T. A., & Seredenin, S. B. (2002). The original novel nootropic and neuroprotective agent Noopept. Eksperimental'naia i klinicheskaia farmakologiia, 65(5), 66-72. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12596521
  79. Heishman, S. J., Kleykamp, B. A., & Singleton, E. G. (2010). Meta-analysis of the acute effects of nicotine and smoking on human performance. Psychopharmacology, 210(4), 453-469. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-010-1848-1
  80. Müller, U., Steffenhagen, N., Regenthal, R., & Bublak, P. (2004). Effects of modafinil on working memory processes in humans. Psychopharmacology, 177(1-2), 161-169. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-004-1926-3
  81. Kjærsgaard, T. (2015). Enhancing motivation by use of prescription stimulants: The ethics of motivation enhancement. AJOB Neuroscience, 6(1), 4-10. https://doi.org/10.1080/21507740.2014.990543
  82. 82.0 82.1 Ilieva, I. P., & Farah, M. J. (2013). Enhancement stimulants: perceived motivational and cognitive advantages. Frontiers in neuroscience, 7, 198. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2013.00198
  83. Nyholm, S. (2015). Motivation-enhancements and domain-specific values. AJOB Neuroscience, 6(1), 37-39. https://doi.org/10.1080/21507740.2014.995313
  84. Terbeck, S. (2013). Why students bother taking Adderall: Measurement validity of self-reports. AJOB Neuroscience, 4(1), 21-22. https://doi.org/10.1080/21507740.2012.762064
  85. Sedler, M. J. (1995). Understanding delusions. Psychiatric Clinics, 18(2), 251-262. https://doi.org/10.4103/0972-6748.57851
  86. Ideas of Reference | Encyclopedia of Psychology (PsychCentral) | http://psychcentral.com/encyclopedia/ideas-of-reference/
  87. 87.0 87.1 87.2 Sjoberg, B. M., & Hollister, L. E. (1965). The effects of psychotomimetic drugs on primary suggestibility. Psychopharmacologia, 8(4), 251-262. ttps://doi.org/10.1007/BF00407857
  88. What is suggestibility? (Psychology Dictionary) | https://psychologydictionary.org/suggestibility/
  89. Carhart-Harris, R. L., Kaelen, M., Whalley, M. G., Bolstridge, M., Feilding, A., & Nutt, D. J. (2015). LSD enhances suggestibility in healthy volunteers. Psychopharmacology, 232(4), 785-794. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-014-3714-z
  90. Kelly, S. F., Fisher, S., & Kelly, R. J. (1978). Effects of cannabis intoxication on primary suggestibility. Psychopharmacology, 56(2), 217-219. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00431853
  91. Cheong, S. H., Lee, K. M., Lim, S. H., Cho, K. R., Kim, M. H., Ko, M. J., ... & Lee, S. E. (2011). The effect of suggestion on unpleasant dreams induced by ketamine administration. Anesthesia & Analgesia, 112(5), 1082-1085. https://doi.org/10.1213/ANE.0b013e31820eeb0e
  92. Whalley, M. G., & Brooks, G. B. (2009). Enhancement of suggestibility and imaginative ability with nitrous oxide. Psychopharmacology, 203(4), 745-752.https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-008-1424-0
  93. Racing Thoughts (yourdictionary) | http://www.yourdictionary.com/racing-thoughts
  94. Pronin, E., Jacobs, E., & Wegner, D. M. (2008). Psychological effects of thought acceleration. Emotion, 8(5), 597. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0013268
  95. Yang, K., Friedman-Wheeler, D. G., & Pronin, E. (2014). Thought acceleration boosts positive mood among individuals with minimal to moderate depressive symptoms. Cognitive therapy and research, 38(3), 261-269. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-014-9597-9
  96. Pronin, E., & Jacobs, E. (2008). Thought speed, mood, and the experience of mental motion. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3(6), 461-485. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-6924.2008.00091.x
  97. How to Stop Racing Thoughts From Anxiety (calm clinic) | https://www.calmclinic.com/anxiety/symptoms/racing-thoughts
  98. Wakefulness Definition | http://www.dictionary.com/browse/wakefulness
  99. Caldwell, J. A., Caldwell, J. L., Smyth, N. K., & Hall, K. K. (2000). A double-blind, placebo-controlled investigation of the efficacy of modafinil for sustaining the alertness and performance of aviators: a helicopter simulator study. Psychopharmacology, 150(3), 272-282. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10923755
  100. FDA Provigil Prescription Guidelines | https://www.accessdata.fda.gov/drugsatfda_docs/label/2015/020717s037s038lbl.pdf
  101. FDA Nuvigil Prescription Guidelines | https://www.accessdata.fda.gov/drugsatfda_docs/label/2015/021875s021lbledt.pdf